[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  PRISMA Flow Diagram
PRISMA Flow Diagram
Figure 2.  Meta-analysis of the Association Between Physician Depressive Symptoms and Medical Errors
Meta-analysis of the Association Between Physician Depressive Symptoms and Medical Errors

The size of squares is proportional to the weight of each study. Horizontal lines indicate the 95% CI of each study; diamond, the pooled estimate with 95% CI; N, the number of participants at baseline; and RR, relative risk.

Figure 3.  Meta-analyses of Long-term Studies of the Association Between Physician Depressive Symptoms and Medical Errors
Meta-analyses of Long-term Studies of the Association Between Physician Depressive Symptoms and Medical Errors

The size of squares is proportional to the weight of each study. Horizontal lines indicate the 95% CI of relative risk (RR) estimate in each study; diamonds, the pooled estimate with 95% CI; and N, the number of participants at baseline.

Table.  Selected Characteristics of the 11 Included Studies
Selected Characteristics of the 11 Included Studies
1.
Institute of Medicine Committee on Quality of Health Care in America; Kohn  LT, Corrigan  JM, Donaldson  MS, eds.  To Err Is Human: Building a Safer Health System. Vol 6. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2000.
2.
James  JT.  A new, evidence-based estimate of patient harms associated with hospital care.  J Patient Saf. 2013;9(3):122-128. doi:10.1097/PTS.0b013e3182948a69PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Makary  MA, Daniel  M.  Medical error-the third leading cause of death in the US.  BMJ. 2016;353:i2139. doi:10.1136/bmj.i2139PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Weingart  SN, Wilson  RM, Gibberd  RW, Harrison  B.  Epidemiology of medical error.  BMJ. 2000;320(7237):774-777. doi:10.1136/bmj.320.7237.774PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Jha  AK, Larizgoitia  I, Audera-Lopez  C, Prasopa-Plaizier  N, Waters  H, Bates  DW.  The global burden of unsafe medical care: analytic modelling of observational studies.  BMJ Qual Saf. 2013;22(10):809-815. doi:10.1136/bmjqs-2012-001748PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Johnson  WG, Brennan  TA, Newhouse  JP,  et al.  The economic consequences of medical injuries: implications for a no-fault insurance plan.  JAMA. 1992;267(18):2487-2492. doi:10.1001/jama.1992.03480180073032PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Bates  DW, Spell  N, Cullen  DJ,  et al; Adverse Drug Events Prevention Study Group.  The costs of adverse drug events in hospitalized patients.  JAMA. 1997;277(4):307-311. doi:10.1001/jama.1997.03540280045032PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Thomas  EJ, Studdert  DM, Newhouse  JP,  et al.  Costs of medical injuries in Utah and Colorado.  Inquiry. 1999;36(3):255-264. PubMedGoogle Scholar
9.
Van Den Bos  J, Rustagi  K, Gray  T, Halford  M, Ziemkiewicz  E, Shreve  J.  The $17.1 billion problem: the annual cost of measurable medical errors.  Health Aff (Millwood). 2011;30(4):596-603. doi:10.1377/hlthaff.2011.0084PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Mata  DA, Ramos  MA, Bansal  N,  et al.  Prevalence of depression and depressive symptoms among resident physicians: a systematic review and meta-analysis.  JAMA. 2015;314(22):2373-2383. doi:10.1001/jama.2015.15845PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Rotenstein  LS, Ramos  MA, Torre  M,  et al.  Prevalence of depression, depressive symptoms, and suicidal ideation among medical students: a systematic review and meta-analysis.  JAMA. 2016;316(21):2214-2236. doi:10.1001/jama.2016.17324PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Fahrenkopf  AM, Sectish  TC, Barger  LK,  et al.  Rates of medication errors among depressed and burnt out residents: prospective cohort study.  BMJ. 2008;336(7642):488-491. doi:10.1136/bmj.39469.763218.BEPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
West  CP, Tan  AD, Habermann  TM, Sloan  JA, Shanafelt  TD.  Association of resident fatigue and distress with perceived medical errors.  JAMA. 2009;302(12):1294-1300. doi:10.1001/jama.2009.1389PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Sen  S, Kranzler  HR, Krystal  JH,  et al.  A prospective cohort study investigating factors associated with depression during medical internship.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(6):557-565. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.41PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Sen  S, Kranzler  HR, Didwania  AK,  et al.  Effects of the 2011 duty hour reforms on interns and their patients: a prospective longitudinal cohort study.  JAMA Intern Med. 2013;173(8):657-662. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.351PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
de Oliveira  GS  Jr, Chang  R, Fitzgerald  PC,  et al.  The prevalence of burnout and depression and their association with adherence to safety and practice standards: a survey of United States anesthesiology trainees.  Anesth Analg. 2013;117(1):182-193. doi:10.1213/ANE.0b013e3182917da9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Heyhoe  J, Birks  Y, Harrison  R, O’Hara  JK, Cracknell  A, Lawton  R.  The role of emotion in patient safety: are we brave enough to scratch beneath the surface?  J R Soc Med. 2016;109(2):52-58. doi:10.1177/0141076815620614PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Frank  E, Prien  RF, Jarrett  RB,  et al.  Conceptualization and rationale for consensus definitions of terms in major depressive disorder: remission, recovery, relapse, and recurrence.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48(9):851-855. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810330075011PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
DeRubeis  RJ, Siegle  GJ, Hollon  SD.  Cognitive therapy versus medication for depression: treatment outcomes and neural mechanisms.  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(10):788-796. doi:10.1038/nrn2345PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Oquendo  MA, Bernstein  CA, Mayer  LES.  A key differential diagnosis for physicians-major depression or burnout [published online July 17, 2019]?.  JAMA Psychiatry. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.1332PubMedGoogle Scholar
21.
Pereira-Lima  K, Gupta  RR, Guille  C, Sen  S.  Residency program factors associated with depressive symptoms in internal medicine interns: a prospective cohort study.  Acad Med. 2019;94(6):869-875. doi:10.1097/ACM.0000000000002567PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Guille  C, Frank  E, Zhao  Z,  et al.  Work-family conflict and the sex difference in depression among training physicians.  JAMA Intern Med. 2017;177(12):1766-1772. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2017.5138PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Tsutsumi  A, Kawanami  S, Horie  S.  Effort-reward imbalance and depression among private practice physicians.  Int Arch Occup Environ Health. 2012;85(2):153-161. doi:10.1007/s00420-011-0656-1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Center  C, Davis  M, Detre  T,  et al.  Confronting depression and suicide in physicians: a consensus statement.  JAMA. 2003;289(23):3161-3166. doi:10.1001/jama.289.23.3161PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Petrie  K, Crawford  J, Baker  STE,  et al.  Interventions to reduce symptoms of common mental disorders and suicidal ideation in physicians: a systematic review and meta-analysis.  Lancet Psychiatry. 2019;6(3):225-234. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30509-1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Moher  D, Liberati  A, Tetzlaff  J, Altman  DG; PRISMA Group.  Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement.  Open Med. 2009;3(3):e123-e130.PubMedGoogle Scholar
27.
Zhang  J, Yu  KF.  What’s the relative risk? a method of correcting the odds ratio in cohort studies of common outcomes.  JAMA. 1998;280(19):1690-1691. doi:10.1001/jama.280.19.1690PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Schünemann  H.  The GRADE Handbook. London, UK: Cochrane Collaboration; 2013.
29.
Löwe  B, Spitzer  RL, Gräfe  K,  et al.  Comparative validity of three screening questionnaires for DSM-IV depressive disorders and physicians’ diagnoses.  J Affect Disord. 2004;78(2):131-140. doi:10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00237-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Higgins  JPT, Thompson  SG, Deeks  JJ, Altman  DG.  Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses.  BMJ. 2003;327(7414):557-560. doi:10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Higgins  JPT, Thompson  SG.  Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis.  Stat Med. 2002;21(11):1539-1558. doi:10.1002/sim.1186PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Sterne  JAC, Jüni  P, Schulz  KF, Altman  DG, Bartlett  C, Egger  M.  Statistical methods for assessing the influence of study characteristics on treatment effects in ‘meta-epidemiological’ research.  Stat Med. 2002;21(11):1513-1524. doi:10.1002/sim.1184PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
van Houwelingen  HC, Arends  LR, Stijnen  T.  Advanced methods in meta-analysis: multivariate approach and meta-regression.  Stat Med. 2002;21(4):589-624. doi:10.1002/sim.1040PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Egger  M, Davey Smith  G, Schneider  M, Minder  C.  Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test.  BMJ. 1997;315(7109):629-634. doi:10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Sterne  JAC, Egger  M.  Funnel plots for detecting bias in meta-analysis: guidelines on choice of axis.  J Clin Epidemiol. 2001;54(10):1046-1055. doi:10.1016/S0895-4356(01)00377-8.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
R Development Core Team. The R Project For Statistical Computing. http://www.R-project.org. Accessed August 1, 2019.
37.
Schwarzer  G.  meta: an R package for meta-analysis.  R News. 2007;7(3):40-45.Google Scholar
38.
Viechtbauer  W.  Conducting meta-analysis in R with the metafor package.  J Stat Softw. 2010;36(3):1-48. doi:10.18637/jss.v036.i03Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Hayashino  Y, Utsugi-Ozaki  M, Feldman  MD, Fukuhara  S.  Hope modified the association between distress and incidence of self-perceived medical errors among practicing physicians: prospective cohort study.  PLoS One. 2012;7(4):e35585. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0035585PubMedGoogle Scholar
40.
Kalmbach  DA, Arnedt  JT, Song  PX, Guille  C, Sen  S.  Sleep disturbance and short sleep as risk factors for depression and perceived medical errors in first-year residents.  Sleep. 2017;40(3):zsw073. doi:10.1093/sleep/zsw073PubMedGoogle Scholar
41.
Kang  E-K, Lihm  H-S, Kong  E-H.  Association of intern and resident burnout with self-reported medical errors.  Korean J Fam Med. 2013;34(1):36-42. doi:10.4082/kjfm.2013.34.1.36PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Shanafelt  TD, Balch  CM, Bechamps  G,  et al.  Burnout and medical errors among American surgeons.  Ann Surg. 2010;251(6):995-1000. doi:10.1097/SLA.0b013e3181bfdab3PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Tawfik  DS, Profit  J, Morgenthaler  TI,  et al.  Physician burnout, well-being, and work unit safety grades in relationship to reported medical errors.  Mayo Clin Proc. 2018;93(11):1571-1580. doi:10.1016/j.mayocp.2018.05.014PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
West  CP, Huschka  MM, Novotny  PJ,  et al.  Association of perceived medical errors with resident distress and empathy: a prospective longitudinal study.  JAMA. 2006;296(9):1071-1078. doi:10.1001/jama.296.9.1071PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Topp  CW, Østergaard  SD, Søndergaard  S, Bech  P.  The WHO-5 well-being index: a systematic review of the literature.  Psychother Psychosom. 2015;84(3):167-176. doi:10.1159/000376585PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Henkel  V, Mergl  R, Kohnen  R, Maier  W, Möller  H, Hegerl  U.  The WHO-5 wellbeing index performed the best in screening for depression in primary care.  BMJ. 2003;8(5):155. doi:10.1159/000376585Google Scholar
47.
Primack  BA.  The WHO-5 Wellbeing Index performed the best in screening for depression in primary care.  ACP J Club. 2003;139(2):48. doi:10.1136/ebm.8.5.155PubMedGoogle Scholar
48.
Mergl  R, Seidscheck  I, Allgaier  AK, Möller  HJ, Hegerl  U, Henkel  V.  Depressive, anxiety, and somatoform disorders in primary care: prevalence and recognition.  Depress Anxiety. 2007;24(3):185-195. doi:10.1002/da.20192PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Saipanish  R, Lotrakul  M, Sumrithe  S.  Reliability and validity of the Thai version of the WHO-Five Well-Being Index in primary care patients.  Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2009;63(2):141-146. doi:10.1111/j.1440-1819.2009.01933.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Panagioti  M, Geraghty  K, Johnson  J,  et al.  Association between physician burnout and patient safety, professionalism, and patient satisfaction: a systematic review and meta-analysis.  JAMA Intern Med. 2018;178(10):1317-1330. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2018.3713PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Schwenk  TL, Gold  KJ.  Physician burnout—a serious symptom, but of what?  JAMA. 2018;320(11):1109-1110. doi:10.1001/jama.2018.11703PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Norton  EC, Dowd  BE.  Log odds and the interpretation of logit models.  Health Serv Res. 2018;53(2):859-878. doi:10.1111/1475-6773.12712PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Norton  EC, Dowd  BE, Maciejewski  ML.  Odds ratios—current best practice and use.  JAMA. 2018;320(1):84-85. doi:10.1001/jama.2018.6971PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Bodenheimer  T, Sinsky  C.  From triple to quadruple aim: care of the patient requires care of the provider.  Ann Fam Med. 2014;12(6):573-576. doi:10.1370/afm.1713PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Sikka  R, Morath  JM, Leape  L.  The Quadruple Aim: care, health, cost and meaning in work.  BMJ Qual Saf. 2015;24(10):608-610. doi:10.1136/bmjqs-2015-004160PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
West  CP.  Physician well-being: expanding the triple aim.  J Gen Intern Med. 2016;31(5):458-459. doi:10.1007/s11606-016-3641-2PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Wallace  JE, Lemaire  JB, Ghali  WA.  Physician wellness: a missing quality indicator.  Lancet. 2009;374(9702):1714-1721. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61424-0PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Kay  M, Mitchell  G, Clavarino  A, Doust  J.  Doctors as patients: a systematic review of doctors’ health access and the barriers they experience.  Br J Gen Pract. 2008;58(552):501-508. doi:10.3399/bjgp08X319486PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Guille  C, Speller  H, Laff  R, Epperson  CN, Sen  S.  Utilization and barriers to mental health services among depressed medical interns: a prospective multisite study.  J Grad Med Educ. 2010;2(2):210-214. doi:10.4300/JGME-D-09-00086.1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Agency for Healthcare Research Quality. National scorecard on rates of hospital-acquired conditions 2010 to 2015: interim data from National Efforts to Make Health Care Safer. https://www.ahrq.gov/hai/pfp/2015-interim.html. Published December 2016. Accessed September 20, 2019.
61.
Weingart  SN, Callanan  LD, Ship  AN, Aronson  MD.  A physician-based voluntary reporting system for adverse events and medical errors.  J Gen Intern Med. 2001;16(12):809-814. doi:10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.10231.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Kontopantelis  E, Springate  DA, Reeves  D.  A re-analysis of the Cochrane Library data: the dangers of unobserved heterogeneity in meta-analyses.  PLoS One. 2013;8(7):e69930. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0069930PubMedGoogle Scholar
3 Comments for this article
EXPAND ALL
Misuse of Terms Diminishes Value of Report
Louise B. Andrew, MD, JD | no affiliation
This well-intentioned effort to shed more light on the potential relationship between physician depression and medical errors actually further confuses the issue, by inaccurately interchanging key terms such as “medical errors” vs “perceived errors” and “depressive symptoms” vs “depression” throughout the analysis, including the title.

Because of this imprecision, both lay and popular medical news writers have already begun to propagate the attention-grabbing (but misleading) headline “Physician depression leads to medical errors”.

91% of the studies measured “perceived” or self reported, as opposed to objective measures of error. This is of particular concern
when those reporting are also experiencing depressive symptoms. Such self-reporters of medical errors are more likely to recall negative events and to view themselves in a negative light, and are therefore more likely to assess a clinical event with a suboptimal outcome as an error, and to blame themselves for that error. Self-reported errors thus may reflect differences in reporting and self-judgment—not actual differences in objective error rates. Only 101 of the 21,517 participants had any kind of external identification of error. In all of the others, the existence of error was assumed from retrospective self report.

The unfortunate conflation of perceived and actual errors throughout the present article is mysteriously justified by reference to a single study purporting to show that “self-reported errors have been found to be highly correlated with recorded events”. That study (Weingart SN, Callanan LD, Ship AN, Aronson MD. A physician-based voluntary reporting system for adverse events and medical errors. J Gen Intern Med. 2001;16(12):809-814 doi:10), a resident peer survey of purported errors, actually illustrates no such thing as it involved residents reporting all errors observed on a service, and NOT personal errors.

The studies included in the meta-analysis use several population screeners for depressive symptoms, highly sensitive but not specific for a diagnosis of depression. Most screens were not followed by any definitive testing or clinical interviews. Yet throughout the paper, the terms “depression” and “depressive symptoms” are regularly conflated. This confuses the issue of whether the physicians being surveyed met clinical criteria for depression, or whether they were experiencing symptoms related instead to burnout, exhaustion, poor sleep, or high stress. Further, while depression screeners assess recent symptoms (typically several weeks), elicitation of self error reports typically covered much longer periods (3-12 months), so the two phenomena being collated, may not even have overlapped in time.

While true that “a reliable estimate of the degree to which physicians with a positive screening for depression are at higher risk for medical errors would be useful”, the paper does not seem to have provided such, instead offering a review on how often physicians with depressive symptoms self-report perceived errors. Such reports may or may not relate to the actual incidence of errors.

Finally, it is disappointing to see the IOM and Makary papers regarding generic adverse events in hospitalized patients cited in support of studies purporting to relate to physician medical errors. Continued amplification of these papers (Mazer and Nabhan, J Gen Intern Med 34(10):2264–7 DOI: 10.1007/s11606-019-05156-7) in the popular press could sustain misperceptions of the roots of the problem.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST: None Reported
READ MORE
The Devil is in the Details
Stephen Strum, MD, FACP | Community Practice of Hematology-Oncology
First, the comment by Louise Andrew MD, JD is an excellent comment and informs the reader more accurately than this meta-analysis by Pereira-Lima et al. I am old school and love technology, but what I find in reviewing meta-analyses is that they are far from being a substitute for an actual investigation that is based on a review of the medical records. Using "keywords" or Medicare billing codes or some other surrogate marker is too glib and often too misleading a way to conduct research. At the very least, take a subset of the patients (in this case physicians) involved and look into the details to see if they correlate with the conclusions of the meta-analysis.

What I have seen in the peer-reviewed literature in the last few decades is what I have evidenced in medicine in general i.e., a move towards fast-food medicine, or what I call McMedicine, and dispensing of the leg-work, the cognitive effort that goes into a sincere assessment. This is echoed not only in publications but in the care of patients, where the history and physical examination has been abridged by copy-paste of the EHR, where there is less analytic (cognitive) thought and instead reliance on laboratory testing and imaging. These short-cuts in medicine, be they in publications or in care, degrade the magnificent essence of science & the medical care of our patients.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST: None Reported
READ MORE
Depressed physicians inherently more honest?
Harald Aanning |
In my experience, physicians - especially surgeons - who we characterize as having some anxiety, mood swings, etc., are much more open about their complications - errors, etc., and would be more likely to self-report than the always confident surgeon(s)...
CONFLICT OF INTEREST: None Reported
Original Investigation
Health Policy
November 27, 2019

Asociación entre los síntomas de depresión en médicos y los errores médicos:Revisión sistemática y metaanálisis

Author Affiliations
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, Federal University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
  • 3Department of Pathology, Memorial Sloan Kettering Cancer Center, New York, New York
  • 4Department of Neuroscience and Behavior, University of São Paulo, Ribeirão Preto, São Paulo, Brazil
JAMA Netw Open. 2019;2(11):e1916097. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.16097
Puntos claveEnglish 中文 (chinese)

Pregunta  ¿Cuál es la magnitud y la dirección de las asociaciones entre los síntomas de depresión en médicos y los errores médicos?

Conclusiones  En esta revisión sistemática y metaanálisis de 11 estudios en los que participaron 21 517 médicos, aquellos médicos con pruebas de detección positivas para la depresión tenían muchas probabilidades de reportar errores médicos. El examen de estudios longitudinales demostró que la asociación entre los síntomas de depresión en médicos y los errores médicos es bidireccional.

Significado  Este estudio concluyó que los síntomas de depresión en médicos se asociaron con los errores médicos y destacó la relevancia del bienestar del médico para la calidad de la atención médica; además, subrayó la necesidad de realizar esfuerzos sistemáticos para prevenir o reducir los síntomas de depresión entre los médicos.

Abstract

Importance  Depression is highly prevalent among physicians and has been associated with increased risk of medical errors. However, questions regarding the magnitude and temporal direction of these associations remain open in recent literature.

Objective  To provide summary relative risk (RR) estimates for the associations between physician depressive symptoms and medical errors.

Data Sources  A systematic search of Embase, ERIC, PubMed, PsycINFO, Scopus, and Web of Science was performed from database inception to December 31, 2018.

Study Selection  Peer-reviewed empirical studies that reported on a valid measure of physician depressive symptoms associated with perceived or observed medical errors were included. No language restrictions were applied.

Data Extraction and Synthesis  Study characteristics and RR estimates were extracted from each article. Estimates were pooled using random-effects meta-analysis. Differences by study-level characteristics were estimated using subgroup meta-analysis and metaregression. The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) guideline was followed.

Main Outcomes and Measures  Relative risk estimates for the associations between physician depressive symptoms and medical errors.

Results  In total, 11 studies involving 21 517 physicians were included. Data were extracted from 7 longitudinal studies (64%; with 5595 individuals) and 4 cross-sectional studies (36%; with 15 922 individuals). The overall RR for medical errors among physicians with a positive screening for depression was 1.95 (95% CI, 1.63-2.33), with high heterogeneity across the studies (χ2 = 49.91; P < .001; I2 = 82%; τ2 = 0.06). Among the variables assessed, study design explained the most heterogeneity across studies, with lower RR estimates associated with medical errors in longitudinal studies (RR, 1.62; 95% CI, 1.43-1.84; χ2 = 5.77; P = .33; I2 = 13%; τ2 < 0.01) and higher RR estimates in cross-sectional studies (RR, 2.51; 95% CI, 2.20-2.83; χ2 = 5.44; P = .14; I2 = 45%; τ2 < 0.01). Similar to the results for the meta-analysis of physician depressive symptoms associated with subsequent medical errors, the meta-analysis of 4 longitudinal studies (involving 4462 individuals) found that medical errors associated with subsequent depressive symptoms had a pooled RR of 1.67 (95% CI, 1.48-1.87; χ2 = 1.85; P = .60; I2 = 0%; τ2 = 0), suggesting that the association between physician depressive symptoms and medical errors is bidirectional.

Conclusions and Relevance  Results of this study suggest that physicians with a positive screening for depressive symptoms are at higher risk for medical errors. Further research is needed to evaluate whether interventions to reduce physician depressive symptoms could play a role in mitigating medical errors and thus improving physician well-being and patient care.

Introduction

Medical errors are a major source of patient harm. Studies estimate that, in the United States, as many as 98 000 to 251 000 hospitalized patients die each year as result of a preventable adverse event.1-4 In addition, medical errors are a major source of morbidity5 and account for billions of dollars in financial losses to health care systems every year.6-9

Depressive symptoms are highly prevalent among physicians,10,11 and several studies have investigated the associations between physician depressive symptoms and medical errors.12-16 Although most studies on physician depressive symptoms and medical errors have identified a substantial association, their results are not unanimous, and questions regarding the direction of these associations remain open in recent literature.17

Depressive symptoms have well-established clinical criteria, and a large body of work has demonstrated that depression is a preventable and treatable condition.18-20 Several studies with physicians have identified potential individual and work environment sources of interventions to prevent the development of depressive symptoms among these professionals,21-24 and although scarce, research on the efficacy of interventions to reduce depressive symptoms in physicians has shown positive results.25

Given that depression is preventable and treatable, a reliable estimate of the degree to which physicians with a positive screening for depression are at higher risk for medical errors would be useful. Such an estimate would inform public health decision-making on strategies to improve patient safety and physician well-being. In this systematic review and meta-analysis, we investigated whether physician depressive symptoms were associated with medical errors. We also examined longitudinal studies to investigate the temporal associations between depressive symptoms and medical errors.

Methods
Search Strategy and Study Eligibility

Two of us (K.P.-L. and L.M.B.) independently identified cross-sectional and longitudinal studies published before December 31, 2018, that reported on the associations between physician depressive symptoms and perceived or objectively assessed medical errors. We systematically searched Embase, ERIC, PubMed, PsycINFO, Scopus, and Web of Science. In addition, guided by the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA),26 we screened the reference lists of the articles and corresponded with study investigators. The search strategy we used was initially designed by the corresponding author (K.P.-L.), and critical revisions and edits to this design were provided by a multiprofessional team of researchers with expertise in conducting systematic reviews and meta-analyses on physician depression (D.A.M., S.S.) and mental health (S.R.L., J.A.C., S.S.) research. The Ribeirão Preto Medical School Institutional Review Board deemed this study exempt from approval and informed consent because it collected and synthesized nonidentifiable data from previously published studies.

For the database searches, terms related to physicians and depressive symptoms were combined with terms related to medical errors, without language restriction; full details of the search strategy are provided in the eMethods in the Supplement. References identified from database searches were exported to EndNote (Clarivate Analytics). After removal of duplicates, full-text articles were obtained if their abstracts were considered to be eligible by at least 1 of us. Each full-text article was assessed independently for final inclusion in this systematic review and meta-analysis, and disagreements were resolved by consensus (we reached 97% overall agreement [113 of 116 articles; κ = 0.87]). Peer-reviewed studies that reported data on perceived or observed medical errors associated with a valid measure of depressive symptoms in practicing and resident physicians (ie, excluding medical students and other health care professionals) were included. Studies that involved both physicians and other health care professionals were included only if they provided separate data for physicians. To be included, studies did not have to consider the association between physician depressive symptoms and medical errors as their primary outcome of interest.

Data Extraction and Quality Assessment

Two of us (K.P.-L., L.M.B.) independently extracted the following data from each article using a standardized study form: (1) study information, including geographic location, survey years, research design, sample size, percentage of respondents among eligible participants, and number of institutions included; (2) characteristics of participants, including mean age, percentage of women, specialties, and career level; and (3) outcomes, including depressive symptoms measure, medical errors question interval, method of medical errors assessment, and data for calculating effect size (eg, relative risk [RR], CIs, P values). The approach recommended by Zhang and Yu27 for converting adjusted odds ratio for RR was used for studies that reported only the results of logistic regression for the associations between physician depressive symptoms and medical errors. Corresponding authors were contacted at least twice when studies did not report enough data to compute the effect size. When studies involved the same population of physicians, only the most comprehensive articles (ie, including those with a greater number of participants or a longer follow-up period) were included.

The methodological quality of the studies was assessed using adapted criteria from the Cochrane Library guidelines.28 Studies were considered methodologically strong or weak on the basis of (1) study design (eg, longitudinal indicated strong; cross-sectional, weak), (2) sample size (≥200 participants indicated strong; <200 participants, weak), (3) ascertainment of depressive symptoms measure (sensitivity and specificity >75% indicated strong; sensitivity and specificity ≤75%, weak), (4) representativeness of the sample (≥2 institutions indicated strong; <2 institutions, weak), and (5) descriptive characteristics of participants (reported data on sex, age, specialties, and career level indicated strong; missing information on sex, age, specialties, or career level, weak). Cutoff scores for sample size, representativeness, and descriptive characteristics were based on thresholds used in previous meta-analyses on physician depression,10,11 whereas cutoff scores for ascertainment of depressive symptoms were based on well-established psychometric quality criteria for depression questionnaires.29 Disagreements regarding quality assessment scores for each individual study were resolved by consensus (with an overall agreement of 98%; κ = 0.96).

Statistical Analysis

Relative risk estimates of physician depressive symptoms associated with medical errors were calculated by pooling study-specific estimates using random-effects models with generic invariance method to incorporate the heterogeneity of the differences across the studies.

Between-study heterogeneity was measured using standard χ2 tests and I2 statistics (values <25% indicate low; 25%-75%, moderate; and >75%, considerable heterogeneity).30,31 Sensitivity analyses were performed by serially excluding each study to determine the implications of individual studies for the pooled RR estimates.

Results from studies grouped according to prespecified study-level characteristics were compared using stratified meta-analysis (for physician career level, specialties included, medical errors question interval, geographic region, depressive symptoms measure, and quality assessment indicators [ie, study design, sample size, ascertainment of the depressive symptoms measure, representativeness of the sample, and descriptive data]) or random-effects metaregression (for year of baseline survey and percentage of women).32,33 To gain insight into the direction of the association between depressive symptoms and medical errors, we calculated pooled RR estimates for longitudinal studies that reported (1) results of physician depressive symptoms associated with subsequent medical errors and (2) RR estimates of medical errors associated with subsequent physician depressive symptoms.

Bias secondary to small study effects was investigated using funnel plots and the Egger test.34,35 We used R, version 3.2.3 (R Project for Statistical Computing),36 with meta37 and metafor38 packages for all analyses. Statistical tests were 2-sided and used a significance threshold of P < .05.

Results
Study Characteristics

Eleven studies involving a total of 21 517 physicians were included in this systematic review and meta-analysis (Figure 1). The characteristics of the included studies are summarized in the Table. A total of 7 studies (64%) were longitudinal (involving 5595 individuals)12-15,39,40,44 and 4 (36%) were cross-sectional (involving 15 922 individuals).16,41-43 Nine studies (82%) took place in the United States,12-16,40,42-44 1 (9%) in Japan,39 and 1 (9%) in South Korea.41 Eight studies (73%) included only training physicians (interns and/or residents),12-16,40,41,44 and 3 (27%) recruited physicians from any career level.39,42,43 Seven studies (64%) recruited physicians from multiple specialties,14,15,39-43 whereas 4 (36%) recruited physicians from a single specialty.12,13,16,44 Among these 4 studies, 1 focused on pediatric residents,12 1 on anesthesiology residents,16 and 2 on internal medicine residents.13,44 The median (interquartile range [IQR]) number of participants per study was 836 (2139). Five studies (46%) assessed depressive symptoms with the 2-item Primary Care Evaluation of Mental Disorders (PRIME-MD-2) questionnaire13,41-44; 3 (27%) used the 9-item Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9)14,15,40; 2 (18%) used the Harvard National Depression Screening Day Scale (HANDS)12,16; and 1 (9%) used the 5-item World Health Organization Well-being Index (WHO-5).39 Sensitivity and specificity commonly reported for these depression instruments are available in eTable 1 in the Supplement.

All but 1 study12 (9%) used self-report measures of medical errors. Eight studies (73%) inquired about medical errors in the past 3 months,13-15,40-44 2 (18%) inquired about medical errors in the past year,16,39 and 1 (9%) actively surveyed medical errors in a 1-month interval.12 Assessment measures and definitions of medical errors adopted by individual studies are available in eTable 2 in the Supplement. Although most studies inquired about major or harmful medical errors,13-16,39,40,42-44 1 study (9%) inquired whether physicians were concerned about errors of any type,41 and 1 study (9%) trained a team of nurses and physicians to collect daily reports of all medication errors occurring on wards and to actively review all medical records and medication orders using structured data forms.12 When evaluated by the established quality assessment criteria, 6 studies (55%) were considered as methodologically strong on the basis of design12-15,39,40,44; 8 (73%), on the basis of sample size13-16,39,40,42,43; 5 (46%), on the basis of ascertainment of depressive symptoms measure12,14-16,40; 8 (73%), on the basis of representativeness of the sample12,14-16,39,40,42,43; and all, on the basis of descriptive characteristics of participants.12-16,39-44 Detailed quality indicators for each study are available in eTable 3 in the Supplement.

Of the 11 included studies, 1 (9%) was used only in the meta-analysis of medical errors associated with subsequent depressive symptoms.44 The reason for excluding this study from the other analyses is that a more recent article reported data on depressive symptoms associated with subsequent medical errors in a more comprehensive sample of physicians.13 Because the more recent study did not report data on medical errors associated with subsequent depressive symptoms, the previous study was included in this directionality meta-analysis and excluded from all other analyses to avoid overlapping data. The approach recommended by Zhang and Yu27 was used for computing RR estimates in 2 studies that reported associations of depressive symptoms and medical errors in the format of an odds ratio.13,44

Associations Between Depressive Symptoms and Medical Errors

Meta-analytic pooling of the associations between depressive symptoms and medical errors yielded a summary RR of 1.95 (95% CI, 1.63-2.33), with high heterogeneity across the studies (χ2 = 49.91; P < .001; I2 = 82%; τ2 = 0.06) (Figure 2). The sensitivity analysis, in which the meta-analysis was serially repeated after exclusion of each study, demonstrated that no individual study had an implication for the overall RR estimate of more than 0.12 points (these estimates varied from 1.85 [95% CI, 1.56-2.19] to 2.07 [95% CI, 1.77-2.43]) (eFigure 1 in the Supplement).

Direction of the Associations

All of the 7 longitudinal studies included in the present review investigated the association of physician depressive symptoms in the next 1,12 3,13-15,40,44 or 12 months.39 One study44 was removed from the first directionality analysis because a later publication, which included a more comprehensive sample, also reported on data regarding depressive symptoms associated with subsequent medical errors.13 Meta-analytic pooling of physician depression associated with medical errors resulted in a pooled RR of 1.62 (95% CI, 1.43-1.84), with low heterogeneity across studies (χ2 = 5.77; P = .33; I2 = 13%; τ2 < 0.01) (Figure 3).

Similarly, 4 of the 7 longitudinal studies provided data on medical errors associated with depressive symptoms in the next 3 months.14,15,40,44 Meta-analytic pooling of these 4 studies (involving 4462 physicians) resulted in a summary RR of 1.67 (95% CI, 1.48-1.87), with low heterogeneity across studies (χ2 = 1.85; P = .60; I2 = 0%; τ2 = 0), suggesting that the association between physician depression and medical errors is bidirectional (Figure 3).

Associations Stratified by Study-Level Characteristics

To identify potential sources of heterogeneity, we performed subgroup meta-analysis of studies stratified by different study-level characteristics when at least 2 studies were available in each comparator subgroup. Studies with exclusively surgical specialties yielded a summary RR estimate that was significantly higher than the summary RR estimate in studies that also included nonsurgical specialties (2.59 [95% CI, 2.10-3.16] vs 1.79 [95% CI, 1.46-3.16]). Furthermore, US studies yielded higher estimates of the association between depression and medical errors compared with non-US studies (2.10 [95% CI, 1.77-2.46] vs 1.39 [95% CI, 1.00-1.93]). Summary RR estimates for studies assessing depressive symptoms through the HANDS or the PRIME-MD-2 were significantly higher compared with the ones identified through the PHQ-9 (HANDS: 2.32 [95% CI, 1.97-2.72]; PRIME-MD-2: 2.39 [95% CI, 1.97-2.86]; PHQ-9: 1.67 [95% CI, 1.45-1.92]) (eFigure 2 in the Supplement). No statistically significant differences in RR estimates were found between subgroups of studies stratified by physician career level or studies inquiring physicians about medical errors in the past 3 or 12 months.

A single study assessed depressive symptoms associated with medication errors actively surveyed in the next month.12 The sensitivity analysis that excluded this study did not show a significant reduction in heterogeneity statistics (from 1.95; 95% CI, 1.63-2.33; χ2 = 49.91; P < .001; I2 = 82%; τ2 = 0.06 to 1.94; 95% CI, 1.61-2.33; χ2 = 49.88; P < .001; I2 = 84%; τ2 = 0.06). In contrast, the sensitivity analysis that excluded the only study39 that used the WHO-5 to assess physician depressive symptoms resulted in a reduction in all heterogeneity statistics (from 1.95; 95% CI, 1.63-2.33; χ2 = 49.91; P < .001; I2 = 82%; τ2 = 0.06 to 2.07; 95% CI, 1.77-2.43; χ2 = 31.91; P < .001; I2 = 75%; τ2 = 0.04) (eFigure 1 in the Supplement). Metaregression results revealed that RR estimates did not significantly vary with baseline survey year (estimate = 0.01; 95% CI, –0.05 to 0.07; QM [statistic for the test of moderators] = 0.14; P = .71) or percentage of female physicians (estimate = –0.06; 95% CI, –1.13 to 1.00; QM = 0.01; P = .91) (eFigure 3 in the Supplement).

When evaluated by the quality assessment indicators, longitudinal studies yielded summary RR estimates that were significantly lower compared with those from the cross-sectional sectional studies (1.62; 95% CI, 1.43-1.84; χ2 = 5.77; P = .33; I2 = 13%; τ2 < 0.01 vs 2.51; 95% CI, 2.20-2.83; χ2 = 5.44; P = .14; I2 = 45%; τ2 < 0.01). No statistically significant differences in RR estimates were found between subgroups of studies stratified by sample size, ascertainment of the depression measure, representativeness of the sample, or descriptive characteristics of the participants (eFigure 4 in the Supplement).

Assessment of Publication Bias

A funnel plot of studies that reported on physician depressive symptoms associated with medical errors is presented in eFigure 5 in the Supplement). The Egger test indicated the absence of significant publication bias (intercept = –2.79; P = .12).

Discussion

This systematic review and meta-analysis of 11 studies involving 21 517 physicians demonstrated an association between physician depressive symptoms and an increased risk for perceived medical errors (RR, 1.95; 95% CI, 1.63-2.33). We also found that the magnitude of the associations of physician depressive symptoms and perceived medical errors were relatively consistent across studies that assessed training and practicing physicians, providing additional evidence that physician depression has implications for the quality of care delivered by physicians at different career stages.

Subgroup meta-analysis of studies stratified by different study-level characteristics identified study design, specialty type, geographic region, and depressive symptoms measure as possible sources of heterogeneity in this meta-analysis. The 6 longitudinal studies that assessed physician depressive symptoms associated with subsequent medical errors yielded a significantly lower summary RR estimate compared with the 4 cross-sectional studies included in this meta-analysis (1.62 [95% CI, 1.43-1.84] vs 2.51 [95% CI, 2.20-2.83]), but a significant increased risk for medical errors among physicians with depressive symptoms was identified in both study designs.

Similarly, although the summary RR estimates for studies that included nonsurgical specialties, that were from non-US countries, and that used the PHQ-9 as a measure of depressive symptoms were significantly lower than the summary RR estimates identified for their reference subgroups, the estimates were still statistically significant for all analyzed subgroups. These results support the main finding that depressive symptoms are associated with an increased risk for medical errors among physicians.

In line with these results, sensitivity analysis demonstrated that no individual study was associated with the overall RR estimate by more than 0.12 points (overall RR estimates in sensitivity analysis varied from 1.85 [95% CI, 1.56-2.19] to 2.07 [95% CI, 1.77-2.43]). The study that accounted for the largest variation in the magnitude of RR estimates (from 1.95 [95% CI, 1.63-2.33] to 2.07 [95% CI, 1.77-2.43]) used the WHO-5 for the ascertainment of depression in Japanese physicians.39 The WHO-5 was originally designed as a measure of subjective well-being and has been validated as a depression screening instrument.45 Studies conducted in primary care settings have suggested that the WHO-5′s broad statements tend to favor sensitivity at the cost of specificity when screening for depression in the general population,46-49 which might have been a source of heterogeneity in the present study.

A previous meta-analysis has associated physician burnout and emotional distress with patient safety outcomes.50 The present meta-analysis advances the findings of this past work in different ways. First, the issue of quantifying heterogeneous constructs of emotional distress in the same meta-analysis was overcome by focusing on depressive symptoms, which have well-established clinical criteria and methods of assessment.20,51 Similarly, by working with RR instead of odds ratio estimates, we were able to more accurately estimate the magnitude of the association between depressive symptoms and perceived medical errors.52,53 Furthermore, the analysis of 7 longitudinal studies12-15,39,40,44 allowed us to demonstrate that physician depressive symptoms are associated with future medical errors (RR, 1.62; 95% CI, 1.43-1.84; n = 5595 physicians from 6 studies12-15,39,40) and that medical errors are associated with future depressive symptoms in physicians (RR, 1.67; 95% CI, 1.48-1.87; n = 4462 physicians from 4 studies14,15,40,44). Taken together, these data suggest that the association between physician depression and medical errors is bidirectional. To our knowledge, this study is the first to systematically review the direction of the associations between physician depressive symptoms and medical errors.

Studies have recommended the addition of physician well-being to the Triple Aim of enhancing the patient experience of care, improving the health of populations, and reducing the per capita cost of health care.54-57 Results of the present study endorse the Quadruple Aim movement by demonstrating not only that medical errors are associated with physician health but also that physician depressive symptoms are associated with subsequent errors. Given that few physicians with depression seek treatment58,59 and that recent evidence has pointed to the lack of organizational interventions aimed at reducing physician depressive symptoms,25 our findings underscore the need for institutional policies to remove barriers to the delivery of evidence-based treatment to physicians with depression. Investments in patient safety have been associated with significant reductions in health care costs,60 and the bidirectional associations between physician depressive symptoms and perceived medical errors verified by this meta-analysis suggest that physician well-being is critical to patient safety. Further studies are needed to explore these associations. Such research should investigate whether systematic interventions for reducing depressive symptoms could be factors in decreased medical errors.

Limitations

This systematic review and meta-analysis has some limitations. First, 10 of 11 studies included relied on self-report measures of medical errors.13-16,39-44 Although substantial differences in RR estimates and heterogeneity statistics were not identified by sensitivity analysis that removed the only study that assessed medical errors through active surveillance,12 the small sample size of the referred study limited its weight in the overall meta-analysis. Furthermore, although self-reported errors have been found to be highly correlated with recorded events,61 the self-report nature of the included studies may have introduced bias to the present results. For instance, physicians with depression may be more likely to perceive medical errors, which may drive the association between depressive symptoms and medical errors. However, the secondary meta-analyses of longitudinal studies that assessed depressive symptoms associated with subsequent medical errors and medical errors associated with future depressive symptoms demonstrated significantly increased risk estimates, which suggests the existence of bidirectional temporal associations between physician depressive symptoms and perceived medical errors. Similarly, all included studies examined and ascertained depressive symptoms from self-report inventories that varied in sensitivity and specificity. Therefore, the results demonstrated the presence of associations between depressive symptoms and perceived medical errors rather than the association between a clinical diagnosis of depression and medical errors.

Second, the 10 studies that evaluated self-reported medical errors included general questions about either major,13-15,39,40,42-44 harmful,16 or any41 medical errors. By doing so, these studies might have underestimated particular acts and omissions with potential to harm that physicians might not have considered to be a major, harmful, or any medical error. In the only study that assessed errors through active surveillance, more than 60% of the observed medical errors were considered to be potentially harmful,12 which suggests that a large portion of medical errors committed by physicians could have negative consequences for patients.

Third, the small number of studies included in some of the subgroups may have biased some of the subgroup analysis results.62 Fourth, despite the significant overall effect of the meta-analytic model of medical errors associated with subsequent depressive symptoms, few studies (4 studies with 4462 physicians)14,15,40,44 were included in this directional analysis, which might also have introduced bias to the results. Fifth, most studies (9 of 11) assessed US physicians.12-16,40,42-44 Therefore, the results may not be generalizable to physicians in other countries.

Sixth, although the 3 studies that evaluated both practicing and training physicians included the largest number of physicians in this meta-analysis (15 327 of 21 517),39,42,43 most of the included studies (8 of 11) exclusively assessed populations of training physicians.12-16,40,41,44 Although the subgroup meta-analysis that stratified studies by physician career level did not identify significant differences between the 2 subgroups, generalizations of the present study results to populations of practicing physicians should be done with caution. Seventh, all references included were from full-text articles published in peer-reviewed journals. Although no evidence of publication bias was verified by Egger test, the exclusion of unpublished data and gray literature might have introduced selection bias to this analysis.

Conclusions

By combining data from multiple studies, this systematic review and meta-analysis found that physician depressive symptoms were associated with increased risk for perceived medical errors and that the association between depressive symptoms and perceived errors was bidirectional. Future research is needed to evaluate the associations of physician depressive symptoms with objective measures of medical errors, such as active surveillance. Studies that include physicians from different countries could answer whether cultural and socioeconomic aspects play a role in the associations between depressive symptoms and errors. Future research is also needed into the degree to which interventions for reducing physician depressive symptoms could mitigate medical errors and improve physician well-being and patient care.

Back to top
Article Information

Accepted for Publication: October 4, 2019.

Published: November 27, 2019. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.16097

Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2019 Pereira-Lima K et al. JAMA Network Open.

Corresponding Author: Karina Pereira-Lima, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan Medical School, 205 Zina Pitcher Pl, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-5720 (pereiralima.k@gmail.com).

Author Contributions: Dr Pereira-Lima had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: Pereira-Lima, Mata, Loureiro, Crippa, Sen.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Pereira-Lima, Loureiro, Crippa, Bolsoni.

Drafting of the manuscript: Pereira-Lima.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors.

Statistical analysis: Pereira-Lima.

Obtained funding: Pereira-Lima, Loureiro, Sen.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Crippa, Bolsoni.

Supervision: Mata, Loureiro, Crippa, Sen.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Crippa reported receiving grants from CNPq (Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico) and CNPq/FAPESP (Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo), other (cannabidiol supply) from BSPG Pharm, and grants from Prati-Donaduzzi Pharmaceutical and University Global Partnership Network outside the submitted work, and reported holding a patent (Mechoulam R, Crippa JA, Guimaraes FS, Zuardi A, Hallak, JE, and Breuer A, inventors. Fluorinated CBD compounds, compositions and uses thereof. Pub. No: WO/2014/108899; International Application No: PCT/IL2014/050023; Def. US no. Reg. 62193296; July 29, 2015; INPI on August 19, 2015 [BR1120150164927] issued; University of São Paulo has an agreement with Prati-Donaduzzi [Toledo, Brazil] to “develop a pharmaceutical product containing synthetic CBD and prove its safety and therapeutic efficacy in the treatment of epilepsy, schizophrenia, Parkinson's disease, and anxiety disorders.”). No other disclosures were reported.

Funding/Support: This study was funded in part by the Arnold P. Gold Foundation Research Institute (Drs Pereira-Lima, Mata, Loureiro, and Sen). Dr Pereira-Lima was supported by research fellowship grants 2016/13410-0 and 2018/21480-4 from FAPESP. Drs Loureiro and Crippa were supported by a productivity fellowship grant from the CNPq. Ms Bolsoni was supported by research fellowship award grant 2016/01801-5 from FAPESP. Dr Sen was supported by grant R01MH101459 from the National Institutes of Health.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Disclaimer: The opinions, results, and conclusions herein are those of the authors and do not reflect the official policy or position of the funding sources.

References
1.
Institute of Medicine Committee on Quality of Health Care in America; Kohn  LT, Corrigan  JM, Donaldson  MS, eds.  To Err Is Human: Building a Safer Health System. Vol 6. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2000.
2.
James  JT.  A new, evidence-based estimate of patient harms associated with hospital care.  J Patient Saf. 2013;9(3):122-128. doi:10.1097/PTS.0b013e3182948a69PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Makary  MA, Daniel  M.  Medical error-the third leading cause of death in the US.  BMJ. 2016;353:i2139. doi:10.1136/bmj.i2139PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Weingart  SN, Wilson  RM, Gibberd  RW, Harrison  B.  Epidemiology of medical error.  BMJ. 2000;320(7237):774-777. doi:10.1136/bmj.320.7237.774PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Jha  AK, Larizgoitia  I, Audera-Lopez  C, Prasopa-Plaizier  N, Waters  H, Bates  DW.  The global burden of unsafe medical care: analytic modelling of observational studies.  BMJ Qual Saf. 2013;22(10):809-815. doi:10.1136/bmjqs-2012-001748PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Johnson  WG, Brennan  TA, Newhouse  JP,  et al.  The economic consequences of medical injuries: implications for a no-fault insurance plan.  JAMA. 1992;267(18):2487-2492. doi:10.1001/jama.1992.03480180073032PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Bates  DW, Spell  N, Cullen  DJ,  et al; Adverse Drug Events Prevention Study Group.  The costs of adverse drug events in hospitalized patients.  JAMA. 1997;277(4):307-311. doi:10.1001/jama.1997.03540280045032PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Thomas  EJ, Studdert  DM, Newhouse  JP,  et al.  Costs of medical injuries in Utah and Colorado.  Inquiry. 1999;36(3):255-264. PubMedGoogle Scholar
9.
Van Den Bos  J, Rustagi  K, Gray  T, Halford  M, Ziemkiewicz  E, Shreve  J.  The $17.1 billion problem: the annual cost of measurable medical errors.  Health Aff (Millwood). 2011;30(4):596-603. doi:10.1377/hlthaff.2011.0084PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Mata  DA, Ramos  MA, Bansal  N,  et al.  Prevalence of depression and depressive symptoms among resident physicians: a systematic review and meta-analysis.  JAMA. 2015;314(22):2373-2383. doi:10.1001/jama.2015.15845PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Rotenstein  LS, Ramos  MA, Torre  M,  et al.  Prevalence of depression, depressive symptoms, and suicidal ideation among medical students: a systematic review and meta-analysis.  JAMA. 2016;316(21):2214-2236. doi:10.1001/jama.2016.17324PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Fahrenkopf  AM, Sectish  TC, Barger  LK,  et al.  Rates of medication errors among depressed and burnt out residents: prospective cohort study.  BMJ. 2008;336(7642):488-491. doi:10.1136/bmj.39469.763218.BEPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
West  CP, Tan  AD, Habermann  TM, Sloan  JA, Shanafelt  TD.  Association of resident fatigue and distress with perceived medical errors.  JAMA. 2009;302(12):1294-1300. doi:10.1001/jama.2009.1389PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Sen  S, Kranzler  HR, Krystal  JH,  et al.  A prospective cohort study investigating factors associated with depression during medical internship.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(6):557-565. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.41PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Sen  S, Kranzler  HR, Didwania  AK,  et al.  Effects of the 2011 duty hour reforms on interns and their patients: a prospective longitudinal cohort study.  JAMA Intern Med. 2013;173(8):657-662. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.351PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
de Oliveira  GS  Jr, Chang  R, Fitzgerald  PC,  et al.  The prevalence of burnout and depression and their association with adherence to safety and practice standards: a survey of United States anesthesiology trainees.  Anesth Analg. 2013;117(1):182-193. doi:10.1213/ANE.0b013e3182917da9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Heyhoe  J, Birks  Y, Harrison  R, O’Hara  JK, Cracknell  A, Lawton  R.  The role of emotion in patient safety: are we brave enough to scratch beneath the surface?  J R Soc Med. 2016;109(2):52-58. doi:10.1177/0141076815620614PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Frank  E, Prien  RF, Jarrett  RB,  et al.  Conceptualization and rationale for consensus definitions of terms in major depressive disorder: remission, recovery, relapse, and recurrence.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48(9):851-855. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810330075011PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
DeRubeis  RJ, Siegle  GJ, Hollon  SD.  Cognitive therapy versus medication for depression: treatment outcomes and neural mechanisms.  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(10):788-796. doi:10.1038/nrn2345PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Oquendo  MA, Bernstein  CA, Mayer  LES.  A key differential diagnosis for physicians-major depression or burnout [published online July 17, 2019]?.  JAMA Psychiatry. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.1332PubMedGoogle Scholar
21.
Pereira-Lima  K, Gupta  RR, Guille  C, Sen  S.  Residency program factors associated with depressive symptoms in internal medicine interns: a prospective cohort study.  Acad Med. 2019;94(6):869-875. doi:10.1097/ACM.0000000000002567PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Guille  C, Frank  E, Zhao  Z,  et al.  Work-family conflict and the sex difference in depression among training physicians.  JAMA Intern Med. 2017;177(12):1766-1772. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2017.5138PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Tsutsumi  A, Kawanami  S, Horie  S.  Effort-reward imbalance and depression among private practice physicians.  Int Arch Occup Environ Health. 2012;85(2):153-161. doi:10.1007/s00420-011-0656-1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Center  C, Davis  M, Detre  T,  et al.  Confronting depression and suicide in physicians: a consensus statement.  JAMA. 2003;289(23):3161-3166. doi:10.1001/jama.289.23.3161PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Petrie  K, Crawford  J, Baker  STE,  et al.  Interventions to reduce symptoms of common mental disorders and suicidal ideation in physicians: a systematic review and meta-analysis.  Lancet Psychiatry. 2019;6(3):225-234. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30509-1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Moher  D, Liberati  A, Tetzlaff  J, Altman  DG; PRISMA Group.  Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement.  Open Med. 2009;3(3):e123-e130.PubMedGoogle Scholar
27.
Zhang  J, Yu  KF.  What’s the relative risk? a method of correcting the odds ratio in cohort studies of common outcomes.  JAMA. 1998;280(19):1690-1691. doi:10.1001/jama.280.19.1690PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Schünemann  H.  The GRADE Handbook. London, UK: Cochrane Collaboration; 2013.
29.
Löwe  B, Spitzer  RL, Gräfe  K,  et al.  Comparative validity of three screening questionnaires for DSM-IV depressive disorders and physicians’ diagnoses.  J Affect Disord. 2004;78(2):131-140. doi:10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00237-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Higgins  JPT, Thompson  SG, Deeks  JJ, Altman  DG.  Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses.  BMJ. 2003;327(7414):557-560. doi:10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Higgins  JPT, Thompson  SG.  Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis.  Stat Med. 2002;21(11):1539-1558. doi:10.1002/sim.1186PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Sterne  JAC, Jüni  P, Schulz  KF, Altman  DG, Bartlett  C, Egger  M.  Statistical methods for assessing the influence of study characteristics on treatment effects in ‘meta-epidemiological’ research.  Stat Med. 2002;21(11):1513-1524. doi:10.1002/sim.1184PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
van Houwelingen  HC, Arends  LR, Stijnen  T.  Advanced methods in meta-analysis: multivariate approach and meta-regression.  Stat Med. 2002;21(4):589-624. doi:10.1002/sim.1040PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Egger  M, Davey Smith  G, Schneider  M, Minder  C.  Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test.  BMJ. 1997;315(7109):629-634. doi:10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Sterne  JAC, Egger  M.  Funnel plots for detecting bias in meta-analysis: guidelines on choice of axis.  J Clin Epidemiol. 2001;54(10):1046-1055. doi:10.1016/S0895-4356(01)00377-8.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
R Development Core Team. The R Project For Statistical Computing. http://www.R-project.org. Accessed August 1, 2019.
37.
Schwarzer  G.  meta: an R package for meta-analysis.  R News. 2007;7(3):40-45.Google Scholar
38.
Viechtbauer  W.  Conducting meta-analysis in R with the metafor package.  J Stat Softw. 2010;36(3):1-48. doi:10.18637/jss.v036.i03Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Hayashino  Y, Utsugi-Ozaki  M, Feldman  MD, Fukuhara  S.  Hope modified the association between distress and incidence of self-perceived medical errors among practicing physicians: prospective cohort study.  PLoS One. 2012;7(4):e35585. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0035585PubMedGoogle Scholar
40.
Kalmbach  DA, Arnedt  JT, Song  PX, Guille  C, Sen  S.  Sleep disturbance and short sleep as risk factors for depression and perceived medical errors in first-year residents.  Sleep. 2017;40(3):zsw073. doi:10.1093/sleep/zsw073PubMedGoogle Scholar
41.
Kang  E-K, Lihm  H-S, Kong  E-H.  Association of intern and resident burnout with self-reported medical errors.  Korean J Fam Med. 2013;34(1):36-42. doi:10.4082/kjfm.2013.34.1.36PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Shanafelt  TD, Balch  CM, Bechamps  G,  et al.  Burnout and medical errors among American surgeons.  Ann Surg. 2010;251(6):995-1000. doi:10.1097/SLA.0b013e3181bfdab3PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Tawfik  DS, Profit  J, Morgenthaler  TI,  et al.  Physician burnout, well-being, and work unit safety grades in relationship to reported medical errors.  Mayo Clin Proc. 2018;93(11):1571-1580. doi:10.1016/j.mayocp.2018.05.014PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
West  CP, Huschka  MM, Novotny  PJ,  et al.  Association of perceived medical errors with resident distress and empathy: a prospective longitudinal study.  JAMA. 2006;296(9):1071-1078. doi:10.1001/jama.296.9.1071PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Topp  CW, Østergaard  SD, Søndergaard  S, Bech  P.  The WHO-5 well-being index: a systematic review of the literature.  Psychother Psychosom. 2015;84(3):167-176. doi:10.1159/000376585PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Henkel  V, Mergl  R, Kohnen  R, Maier  W, Möller  H, Hegerl  U.  The WHO-5 wellbeing index performed the best in screening for depression in primary care.  BMJ. 2003;8(5):155. doi:10.1159/000376585Google Scholar
47.
Primack  BA.  The WHO-5 Wellbeing Index performed the best in screening for depression in primary care.  ACP J Club. 2003;139(2):48. doi:10.1136/ebm.8.5.155PubMedGoogle Scholar
48.
Mergl  R, Seidscheck  I, Allgaier  AK, Möller  HJ, Hegerl  U, Henkel  V.  Depressive, anxiety, and somatoform disorders in primary care: prevalence and recognition.  Depress Anxiety. 2007;24(3):185-195. doi:10.1002/da.20192PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Saipanish  R, Lotrakul  M, Sumrithe  S.  Reliability and validity of the Thai version of the WHO-Five Well-Being Index in primary care patients.  Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2009;63(2):141-146. doi:10.1111/j.1440-1819.2009.01933.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Panagioti  M, Geraghty  K, Johnson  J,  et al.  Association between physician burnout and patient safety, professionalism, and patient satisfaction: a systematic review and meta-analysis.  JAMA Intern Med. 2018;178(10):1317-1330. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2018.3713PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Schwenk  TL, Gold  KJ.  Physician burnout—a serious symptom, but of what?  JAMA. 2018;320(11):1109-1110. doi:10.1001/jama.2018.11703PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Norton  EC, Dowd  BE.  Log odds and the interpretation of logit models.  Health Serv Res. 2018;53(2):859-878. doi:10.1111/1475-6773.12712PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Norton  EC, Dowd  BE, Maciejewski  ML.  Odds ratios—current best practice and use.  JAMA. 2018;320(1):84-85. doi:10.1001/jama.2018.6971PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Bodenheimer  T, Sinsky  C.  From triple to quadruple aim: care of the patient requires care of the provider.  Ann Fam Med. 2014;12(6):573-576. doi:10.1370/afm.1713PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Sikka  R, Morath  JM, Leape  L.  The Quadruple Aim: care, health, cost and meaning in work.  BMJ Qual Saf. 2015;24(10):608-610. doi:10.1136/bmjqs-2015-004160PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
West  CP.  Physician well-being: expanding the triple aim.  J Gen Intern Med. 2016;31(5):458-459. doi:10.1007/s11606-016-3641-2PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Wallace  JE, Lemaire  JB, Ghali  WA.  Physician wellness: a missing quality indicator.  Lancet. 2009;374(9702):1714-1721. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61424-0PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Kay  M, Mitchell  G, Clavarino  A, Doust  J.  Doctors as patients: a systematic review of doctors’ health access and the barriers they experience.  Br J Gen Pract. 2008;58(552):501-508. doi:10.3399/bjgp08X319486PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Guille  C, Speller  H, Laff  R, Epperson  CN, Sen  S.  Utilization and barriers to mental health services among depressed medical interns: a prospective multisite study.  J Grad Med Educ. 2010;2(2):210-214. doi:10.4300/JGME-D-09-00086.1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Agency for Healthcare Research Quality. National scorecard on rates of hospital-acquired conditions 2010 to 2015: interim data from National Efforts to Make Health Care Safer. https://www.ahrq.gov/hai/pfp/2015-interim.html. Published December 2016. Accessed September 20, 2019.
61.
Weingart  SN, Callanan  LD, Ship  AN, Aronson  MD.  A physician-based voluntary reporting system for adverse events and medical errors.  J Gen Intern Med. 2001;16(12):809-814. doi:10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.10231.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Kontopantelis  E, Springate  DA, Reeves  D.  A re-analysis of the Cochrane Library data: the dangers of unobserved heterogeneity in meta-analyses.  PLoS One. 2013;8(7):e69930. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0069930PubMedGoogle Scholar
×